Fighting
the Bite: Critiques of Lyme
Disease Interventions and Proposed Solutions Based on Alternate Behavior
Theories – Michael Gonzales
Introduction
Lyme
disease is an infectious disease that is transmitted through tick bites and was
first identified in 1975. Lyme
disease is currently endemic in 15 states (1), and rates of infection are still
increasin0 rapidly (2). The
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) report that the 2011 national
ratio of Lyme disease infection is 7.8 cases for every 100,000 persons. In the ten states where Lyme disease is
most common, the average was 31.6 cases for every 100,000 persons, and the
highest incidence is 84.6 for every 100,000 persons in Delaware, more than triple
of what it was in 2002 (3). The
most commonly affected from 2001-2010 were children from 5-15 years of age and
adults from 40-50 years of age (4).
Symptoms occur most often in the summer months, with the highest clearly
being in July (2). A combination
of factors, such as increased exposures to ticks due to deforestation to
increased outdoor activities is hypothesized to have contributed to this rise
in infections (2).
Ticks are located in bushes, forests, grass, and generally anywhere there
is humidity and warmth. They cling
to passing mammals and locate an area on the skin to bite. The bite is hard to detect because
ticks are barely visible due to their small size and they also secrete
chemicals that mask the pain and sense of itchiness of the bite (5). This allows the tick to feed for hours,
although transmission of Lyme disease can take as soon as 4 hours (1). Many times, people do not even notice
they were bitten until much later on, if at all. A bite does not necessarily mean the tick has Lyme disease
to transmit. For this reason and
other factors, only 1% of tick bites results in the diagnosis of Lyme disease
(1). However, if left untreated
for several months, symptoms can start to adversely affect the joints, heart,
and central nervous system, potentially leading to permanent physical and
mental disabilities (6).
One major prevention technique is to wear protective clothing while doing
outdoor activities such as hiking.
Clothing includes a hat, long-sleeved shirts and pants to reduce skin
exposure. Light-colored clothing also makes the tick more visible. One can remove a biting tick by removing
it gently using a pair of tweezers (7).
Other prevention methods include pesticides with DEET, treating the yard
to prevent tick infestation, and mammal (particularly rodent and deer)
population control as they are often carriers (7). Unfortunately, there is no single magic bullet to prevent
human cases as geographic distribution continues to expand (2).
The Tick-Borne Disease Alliance (TBDA) is an organization with goals of raising
awareness, supporting initiatives, and promoting advocacy for the eradication
of tick-borne diseases, especially for Lyme disease (8). One of their major means of raising
awareness is through education and organization of various support initiatives
across the nations. They have also
produced videos to this end. This
paper will analyze 3 ways this public campaign can be improved, first by
critiquing 3 of its methods and then proposing 3 solutions.
Critique #1 – Health Belief
Model: Education is not Enough
The Health Belief Model (HBM)
is based on the concept that an individual person weighs the risks and benefits
of a health behavior and plans the action accordingly (9). For example, a hiker will see on the
news that the tick population is increasing on certain trails and has to decide
whether to wear a long-sleeved shirt and pants. HBM predicts that the hiker will base his decision on his
assessment of personal risk, the severity of the consequences if he gets
bitten, any barriers or obstacles to wearing the extra clothing (i.e., getting
too hot), and the positive consequences.
Although this seems like a rational concept, it is greatly limited as it
ignores the significant group and social factors that play into people’s
decisions. Education is a great
tool for prevention, however, TBDA’s campaign is too focused on the idea that
if people simply knew how to avoid ticks and Lyme disease infection, that they
would do it.
Behavioral
risk factors for tick bites include spending more than 5 hours per week on
trails, woodcutting, spending more than 30 hours outdoors per week, and
engaging in outdoor activities for any significant period of time (10). Many of these behaviors are
unavoidable, especially for outdoor workers, and the infection rate age group
distribution makes sense in that kids spend much of their time outdoors in the
summer. So, it is a good idea for
TBDA to focus on preventive measures without relying on people not spending
time outdoors. However, research
into preventive measures and effects has shown that preventive measures do not increase
after the target population had been educated on various techniques (11, 12, 13). The literature also shows evidence that
people do not perform the preventive measures even after knowledge of the
severity of Lyme disease a (12, 13).
Public health campaigns, including TBDA, centered on Lyme disease prevention are not addressing the social
factors.
Unlike
HBM, group behavior models take into account how the beliefs, attitudes, and
actions of other people can affect the individual’s decisions. One relevant concept is the principle
of conformity, whereby the individual models his beliefs, attitudes, and
behaviors to group norms (14). It
can occur in small groups (i.e., group of outdoor employees or hiking group) or
in society as a whole, and even when the individual is alone (14). So, even if a hiker is alone, he would
likely wear what he saw in media, which is almost never full protective
clothing, hat, etc. In fact, if a
hiker does wear such things, he would fear that he is an outsider of the group
as that is how they are often depicted in media. This type of behavior is explained by the Social Cognitive Theory
that explains that much of our learning is from observing others (15). Tied into this is the spotlight effect
or that people think other people are watching them more than they actually are
(16). Individuals will
emulate the behavior of others even if it is discordant with the individual’s
beliefs and education. This theory
would predict that a hiker who has been briefed on all the dangers of tick
bites and how to prevent them, would nonetheless not wear pants and a hat if
the rest of the group didn’t. None
of these social factors are addressed in TBDA’s website or campaign
materials.
Critique #2 – Psychological Reactance: More Harm than Help
Many public health campaign messages are similar
to warnings; “Don’t do this” or “Don’t do that of this will happen to
you”. One line from the TBDA
website states, “Walk in the center of woodland trails, and by all means avoid
walking along deer paths” (8).
This is a rational intervention method, whereby you would want to
disseminate important information and stress the importance of adhering to it. Unfortunately, it does not take into
account psychological reactance.
Psychological
reactance is a reaction to people, ideas, rules, or concepts that threatens a
person’s perceived freedoms (17).
The person who is threatened will react in a predictable way, and that
is to restore their freedom, usually by performing the perceived restrictive
action (18). For this example, one
can imagine that if an authority figure tells kids not to wander from the
trail, there would be some kids who do precisely that in order to restore their
sense of freedom. Even if they
were told about the danger of ticks, this reaction impulse can sometimes
override their rational sense of danger.
An
important part of this theory is that the level of reaction is directly related
to perceived importance of the freedom (18). The restriction of movement would likely fall into most of
the population’s list of biggest violations of freedom. It is common knowledge that telling a
child to stand still for an extended period of time is very difficult. For authority figures, such as parents,
to tell their kids to stay on the trail and not explore would definitely lead
to some psychological reactance.
The choice of what to wear and how to look is also another important
freedom. One can imagine an ad or
campaign telling kids they should wear long socks and a hat on a hike or when
playing outdoors would also lead to reactance. This expands to outdoor occupations as well. A person working outdoor on the job
might see such ads about avoiding ticks and may think, “Don’t tell me how to do
my job”. Again, the freedom of
choosing what to wear is important, especially for adults who work outdoors
where there might not be a strict job code.
One
message from the TBDA campaign is a video from a celebrity (http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=h43O_ruxP7I) (19).
There are a few factors that can have the unintended effect of
psychological reactance. The first
is the statement, “If you don’t look into it more, you can be at risk”, which
sounds like a warning (19).
Research has shown that controlling language does not generate as much
as interest as low-controlling language and this can fall on deaf ears (20). Also, the audience must be able to
relate to the communicator and think that he or she is credible (21). From the video, the celebrity is young
pretty woman who seems like she is not doing any outdoor activity (she is on a
grass field). Hikers, campers,
outdoor workers, etc. will probably not relate to her and think she understands
the inconveniences of protective clothing. Worst of all, she is wearing a shirt and her legs are
exposed in places where ticks probably are! Not only will this lead the audience into thinking she is
not credible and insincere, but might actually lead kids to emulate the
celebrity’s behavior and also be exposed to ticks.
Critique #3 – Optimistic Bias: Discounting Risks
TBDA,
like many Lyme disease brochures, instructions, and campaign materials,
highlights some of the statistics and the significant risks of exposing oneself
to ticks. Although the intention
is to increase attention to the information, TBDA does not take into account
that people, in general, do no intuitively understand probabilities. TBDA has an active Twitter account as
part of their social media campaign (tbdalliance), but some of the “tweets” are
probably not effective such as, “State health departments reported 22,561
confirmed #LymeDisease
cases and 7,597 probable cases to the CDC in 2010”. Although factual, these numbers might seem low to people and
there is no incentive to compare those numbers to other infectious diseases. Research has showed that people, when
making decisions such as what to wear for going outdoors and going off-trail,
do not incorporate probabilities strictly into their decision-making process,
but rather distort them with biases, emotions, and prior beliefs (22). Another aspect of distorted risk
perceptions is that risks with long-term consequences, such as Lyme disease, is
generally discounted for either more immediate risks or pleasure, such as the
risk of appearing as an outsider to your peers or impressing your peers by
appearing “risky” by going off-trail (23). Public campaigns shouldn’t extensively use statistics in
their message as it can be confusing and boring especially for kids who are the
most prevalent age group for Lyme disease.
One
important part of risk perception is the concept of optimistic bias. Optimistic bias is when people believe
they are less at risk than others, and can be caused by their goals and overall
mood (24). One example is smokers
believing they are less likely to get lung cancer than other smokers even when
they have no information on the lifestyles of other people (25). So one can imagine that even when being
told by TBDA that, “Fewer than half of patients with Lyme disease recall a tick
bite…In some studies this number is as low as 15%”, that some people might
react by thinking, “Oh, well that won’t happen to me, of course I’ll know when
I’ll get bitten!” (8).
One
of the messages that TBDA relays through their campaign is that there is almost
no control on who gets Lyme disease.
Ticks can bite you and you might not even notice fast enough to do
anything about it. If they do bite
you, there is uncertainty from doctors on what to do. Unfortunately, the illusion of control plays a factor into
people’s decision making. The
illusion of control is when people overestimate their ability to control events
even when events are entirely due to chance, such as rolling dice (26). This plays into effect here because
instead of saying, “I better wear protective clothing and not take the chance I
get but”, people, under the illusion of control, might say, “It’s ok to wear
shorts, I’ll know where to avoid ticks and I have been camping for a long time
and never got bitten”. To avoid
addressing these illusions and misinterpretations of risks and control can have
the opposite effect and actually contribute to more risky actions.
Proposed
Intervention: Using Framing,
Advertising, and Social Norms to Change Behavior
I
believe TBDA has a great foundation in place, but more focus on behavioral
changes would make for a more effective campaign. The basic premise behind TBDA’s campaign is that if people
just had the information, their behavior would change to reduce the incidence
of Lyme disease. However, research
and theory show that does is not often the case. The proposed intervention will supplement TBDA to address
the flaws and this new intervention will be called “TBDA Plus”. In order to enact behavioral changes, more
than just information dissemination will be needed.
Rather
than state that Lyme disease is a danger and reiterate that preventive measures
will help to avoid it, the main message will be reframed to appeal to the core
values of children and adults. To
that end, a television campaign ad reinforced by social media (TBDA already has
an established Twitter, Facebook, and YouTube account) will air in the
Northeast, where Lyme disease prevalence is highest. A sports figure will be
hired to present the message while decked out in full preventive clothing
(pants, hat, etc.) while hiking to better relate to the audience. These ads will display kids in similar
outfits as well. Instead of
spouting statistics, the sports figure can say that he has the freedom to not
worry about ticks and just enjoy the activity because of his protection
efforts. Showing other adults and
kids without the preventive measures scratching themselves all the time and not
having a good time would also contribute the reframed messages.
In
the most popular camping and hiking areas, a campaign effort that results in
tick prevention trainings throughout the summer will be done. Part of this effort will be giving out
rewards to those adults or kids who complete the training, including a
specialized armband to point out to others that one is an “expert” in tick
detection and prevention. Creating
the impression that people with these armbands are exclusive and special would
make the armband a commodity.
Billboards and signs that point out to other hikers and campers that
they should seek help from these experts would only make the armband, and thus
the training, more wanted.
The
last piece of the campaign will be tied to the homes. Similar to the armbands, the city can inspect homes to see
if it has done sufficient preventive measures to protect against tick
infestations. If so, the home will
be given a sign (similar to a “For Sale” sign, except it could say, “Tick
Free”), the homeowners can proudly display on their yard. That way, neighbors and guests can rest
easy when visiting, especially if the neighbor’s kids come over to play.
Defense #1 –
Reframing: Promise of Freedom
The
current core value of the messages behind TBDA’s campaigns is health. TBDA Plus will reframe the message to
freedom in order to step away from HBM.
This will be done
through campaign televisions promotional ads, print material, and news
interviews. Advertising theory can
help reframe the message through promising the core value of freedom to the
audience and supporting that promise with emotions evoked by the images,
sounds, and narratives.
Advertising theory emphasizes the concept that behavior can be changed
through advertising if the ad promises a benefit and there is sufficient
evidence that the promise will be fulfilled (27). In the case of Lyme disease,
instead of promising health benefits (core value of health), the ads will
promote freedom benefits through the use of preventive techniques against tick
bites. Freedom is a very powerful
core value and the promise of it can be greater than the promise of health. So the commercial that shows a
recognizable athlete hiking care-free promises that if you wear full clothing,
you’ll have more freedom and enjoyment out of your hike. There will be less worrying. How that message will be supported is
the athlete smiling and hiking faster than other people who are visibly itching
themselves, not smiling, and admiring the athlete. This could even tie in secondary benefits such as admiration
and sports performance.
Another example of how advertising theory can
utilize the reframing of health is with home owners. Instead of a celebrity sports figure, a “regular Joe” can be
shown having to stop his kids from playing in the yard because of ticks. As word of Lyme disease spreads in the
summer, he installs woodchips and other preventive techniques around his
property. Soon, the kids are
playing free in the yard with the other neighborhood kids (they could come
running from the houses). The
promise is again freedom for the kids and relief for the parents. The happiness of the kids and parents
are the support. So instead of not
addressing social factors, the TBDA Plus campaign would try to appeal to the
more important values of the audience.
Defense #2 – Reactance Reduction: Changing the Message and the Messenger
In order to reverse psychological reactance, the TBDA Plus campaign would
have to change its message and the messenger. The core value change of the message was already discussed
but more changes need to be done in order to lessen the impact of
reactance. If the messages are
concrete and not controlling, and the messenger credible, this will help to
reduce reactance (20). The
proposed ads will have a concrete message as it will show the actions that
people do that could lead to Lyme disease, such as hiking and playing in the
yard. There will hardly be any
language in the ads as to imply that tick prevention is a choice, albeit a good
choice, so as to not threaten the audience’s freedom. It will show the negative consequences of not performing the
advertised behavior, but it will do so in a subtle way with no demands. In fact, these ads will restore freedom
back to the audience.
TBDA
Plus will also change the messenger to someone more credible. Compliance can also increase if the
messenger is similar to the target audience (21). So, first the main promoter or the people who are happy in
the ad must be enacting the desired behavior. All the happy hikers should wear full clothing and not
wander off the trail. The main
promoter should look like they have been hiking for a long time or is a
professional athlete. Using a
celebrity in this instance is probably not a good idea because this could cause
the impression that he or she is not credible. The audience might say, “What does he know about camping, he
lives in a mansion”. In order to
affirm the idea of similarities, the expert hiker in the group can lead a group
people who obviously not athletes, but just “regular Joes”.
Defense #3 – Using the Illusion: Giving back Control
Since
people tend to overestimate their control of events and underestimate their
personal risk, the use of statistics and probabilities are usually not very
effective. Instead, personal
stories and anecdotes can be used in ads and can be very effective in social
marketing (27). A compelling
narrative should be made that effectively communicates the message of the
dangers of Lyme disease. One
example for adults would be having an adult unknowingly getting bitten by a
tick. He has trouble sleeping and
wakes up very drowsy. He slouches
his way to work, only muttering to co-workers and family and appears
disheveled. Someone says, “Have
some coffee!” and he does and drinks a huge cup of coffee. But it doesn’t work and he’s
shocked. He keeps on drinking and
drinking more coffee but it’s not working. He finally realizes he has Lyme disease and knows he can’t
ever “wake up”. This type of
message is more relatable to the population than the possible symptoms of Lyme
disease.
The
distribution of armbands can be used to fortify a person’s sense of control
over tick bites in several ways. This
makes the illusion of control a non-factor. One way to reduce optimistic bias is to introduce a training
team located at hot spot outdoor activity spots that can give out hats and
other protections. We can use a
Mt. Washington, a popular hiking trail in New Hampshire, as an example. The training team would be composed of
people most similar to the visiting hikers, one group for adults and one group
for kids. They will demonstrate
the difficulty of locating ticks when wearing dark clothing. Research has shown that the more
similar the comparison group is to the individuals, the more likely the
individuals would adopt the group risk as their own (28). This also combats the empathy gap
effect, where people underestimate their risks when in a “cold state” or where,
for this example, they are told the risks of Lyme disease while they are
watching TV and are not outdoors (29).
Having the training group right at the mountain will have greater
influence since the hikers are at their “hot state”. Another benefit is that senses of greater control over their
environment through the wearing of the armband give a person greater control of
his impulses (29).
It
is important to ensure the armband and other clothing is perceived as an item
of value to adults and children.
For children, the training can be a type of game “Whoever finds the most
ticks, etc.”. For adults, it can
give them a sense of control of ticks and an item of prestige to other
parents. These rewards act as a
sort of reinforcement for the desired behaviors of preventing tick bites which
increases compliance (15). Once
some kids see other kids with armbands, they will want it to in order to fit
with the social norms. The initial
goal would be to first get the most recognized hikers in the area to agree to
wear them without public knowledge.
These opinion leaders have the most influence on later adopters and make
it more likely that other will follow suit. The Diffusion of Innovations theory explains the intricacies
behind how innovations are adopted by the public (30). Once the early adopters are set, there
can be a huge increase in adoption, which would be the goal here. This can apply to homeowners with their
signs as well. Research would have
to be done to see who the most popular people are. Once the level of adoption of armbands levels off, new rewards
and recognition items can be handed out, along with more training.
Conclusion
TBDA’s
campaign to prevent Lyme disease through raising awareness is heavily based on
the Heath Belief Model. Research
has shown the people need more than just information in order to change
behavior. In order to reduce Lyme
disease incidences, social factors must be addressed to motivate people to
adopt preventive measures against tick bites. The use of advertising theory, the addressing of
psychological reactance, and giving control back to the people will help to
achieve the campaign’s goals.
References
1.
Steere, A. Lyme Disease. N Engl J Med. 2001; 345:115-125.
2. CDC. Lyme Disease: A Public Information Guide.
Atlanta, GA. CDC. http://www.cdc.gov/lyme/resources/brochure/508_LD_Brochure.pdf
3. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Reported Lyme
disease cases by state, 2000-2010. Atlanta, GA. CDC. http://www.cdc.gov/lyme/stats/chartstables/reportedcases_statelocality.html
4. CDC. Confirmed Lyme
disease cases by age and sex--United States, 2001-2010. Atlanta, GA. CDC. http://www.cdc.gov/lyme/stats/chartstables/incidencebyagesex.html
5.
Fikrig E, Narasimhan S. Borrelia
burgdorferi--traveling incognito? Microbes
Infect. 2006; 8 (5): 1390–9.
6.
Cairns, V, Godwin,
J. Post-Lyme borreliosis
syndrom: a meta-analysis of
reported symptoms. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2005; 34 (6):
1340-1345.
7. Zeller, J, MD, PhD;
Burke, Alison, MA; Glass, R, MD.
Lyme Disease. JAMA. 2007; 297 (23): 2664
9.
Rosenstock, M., Strecher, J., Becker, H. Social Learning Theory and the Health
Belief Model. Health Education & Behavior 1998; 15 (2):
175–183.
10.
Poland, G.A. Prevention of Lyme disease: a review of
literature. Mayo Clinic Proceedings 2001;
76: 713-724.
11.Zibit, M. Components of the Health Belief Model are Determinants of
Lyme Disease Preventive Behavior Among School-Aged Children in an Endemic Area
of Massachusetts. American College of
Rheumatology Annual Scientific Meeting Presentation #1946 2006.
12.
Cartter, M. L., Farley, T. A., Ardito, H. A., &
Hadler, J. L. Lyme disease
prevention—knowledge, beliefs, and behaviors among high school students in an
endemic area. Connecticut Medicine 1989;
53: 354-356.
13.
Shadick, N. A., Daltroy, L. H., Phillips, C. B.,
Liang, U. S., & Liang, M. H. Determinants of tick avoidance behaviors in an
endemic area for Lyme disease. American
Journal of Preventive Medicine 1997; 13: 264-270.
14.
Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. Social influence: Compliance and
conformity. Annual
Review of Psychology 2004; 55:
591–621.
15.Miller, N.E. &
Dollard, J. Social Learning and Imitation. Yale
University Press 1941.
16.
Gilovich, T., Medvec, V. H., & Savitsky, K. The spotlight effect in social
judgment: an egocentric bias in estimates of the salience of one's own actions
and appearance. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology 2000; 78(2): 211–222.
17.
Brehm, J. W.
A theory of psychological reactance. Academic
Press 1996.
18.
Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. Psychological Reactance: A Theory of
Freedom and Control. Academic Press 1981.
19.
Tick-Borne Disease Alliance YouTube Video. Lyme
Disease – Turn the Corner Foundation (now TBDA). New York, NY. http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=h43O_ruxP7I
20.
Miller, C. H., Lane, L. T., Deatrick, L. M., Young,
A. M., & Potts, K. A. Psychological reactance and promotional health
messages: The effects of controlling language, lexical concreteness, and the
restoration of freedom. Human
Communication Research 2007; 33: 219-240.
21.
Silvia, P. J. Deflecting reactance: The role of
similarity in increasing compliance and reducing resistance. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 2005;
27: 277–284.
22.
Tversky, A., Kahneman D. Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science 1974; 185: 1124–1131.
23.
Dietz, K.
The Psychology of Environmental Problems: Psychology for Sustainability.
New York Psychology 2006; 2010:
216-217.
24.
Shepperd, J., Carroll, P., Grace, J., Terry, M. Exploring the Causes of Comparative
Optimism. Psychologica Belgica 2002; 42:
65–98.
25.
Weinstein, N. Klein W. Unrealistic Optimism: Present
and Future. Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology 1996; 15 (1): 1–8.
26.
Thompson, S.
Illusions of Control: How We Overestimate Our Personal Influence Current
Directions in Psychological Science.
Association for Psychological
Science 1999; 8 (6): 187–190.
27.
Grier, S., Bryant, C. Social marketing in public health. Annual
Review of Public Health 2005; 26:319-339.
28.
Perloff, L., Fetzer B. Self-other judgments and perceived vulnerability to
victimization. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology 1986; 50: 502–510.
29.
Nordgren LF, Van Harreveld F, van der Pligt J. The
restraint bias: how the illusion of self-restraint promotes impulsive behavior.
Psychol Sci 2009; 20 (12): 1523–8.
No comments:
Post a Comment